scispace - formally typeset
Search or ask a question
Institution

University of Konstanz

EducationKonstanz, Baden-Württemberg, Germany
About: University of Konstanz is a education organization based out in Konstanz, Baden-Württemberg, Germany. It is known for research contribution in the topics: Population & Membrane. The organization has 12115 authors who have published 27401 publications receiving 951162 citations. The organization is also known as: University of Constance & Universität Konstanz.
Topics: Population, Membrane, Politics, Laser, Gene


Papers
More filters
Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: Empirical event-related brain potentials were recorded as subjects read, without further instruction, consecutively presented sequences of words, providing evidence for robust enhancement of early visual processing of stimuli with learned emotional significance and underscoring the salience of emotional connotations during reading.
Abstract: Electroencephalographic event-related brain potentials were recorded as subjects read, without further instruction, consecutively presented sequences of words. We varied the speed at which the sequences were presented (3 Hz and 1 Hz) and the words' emotional significance. Early event-related cortical responses during reading differentiated pleasant and unpleasant words from neutral words. Emotional words were associated with enhanced brain responses arising in predominantly left occipito-temporal areas 200 to 300 ms after presentation. Emotional words were also spontaneously better remembered than neutral words. The early cortical amplification was stable across 10 repetitions, providing evidence for robust enhancement of early visual processing of stimuli with learned emotional significance and underscoring the salience of emotional connotations during reading. During early processing stages, emotion-related enhancement of cortical activity along the dominant processing pathway is due to arousal, rather than valence of the stimuli. This enhancement may be driven by cortico-amygdaloid connections.

401 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: The present studies investigate the relative importance of medium (traditional vs. cyber), publicity (public vs. private), and bully’s anonymity (anonymous vs. not anonymous) for the perceived severity of hypothetical bullying scenarios among a sample of Swiss seventh- and eight-graders and suggest that the role of medium is secondary to the roles of publicity and anonymity when it comes to evaluating bullying severity.
Abstract: Cyberbullying, a modern form of bullying performed using electronic forms of contact (e.g., SMS, MMS, Facebook, YouTube), has been considered as being worse than traditional bullying in its consequences for the victim. This difference was mainly attributed to some specific aspect that are believed to distinguish cyberbullying from traditional bullying: an increased potential for a large audience, an increased potential for anonymous bullying, lower levels of direct feedback, decreased time and space limits, and lower levels of supervision. The present studies investigated the relative importance of medium (traditional vs. cyber), publicity (public vs. private), and bully’s anonymity (anonymous vs. not anonymous) for the perceived severity of hypothetical bullying scenarios among a sample of Swiss seventh- and eight-graders (study 1: 49 % female, mean age = 13.7; study 2: 49 % female, mean age = 14.2). Participants ranked a set of hypothetical bullying scenarios from the most severe one to the least severe one. The scenarios were experimentally manipulated based on the aspect of medium and publicity (study 1), and medium and anonymity (study 2). Results showed that public scenarios were perceived as worse than private ones, and that anonymous scenarios were perceived as worse than not anonymous ones. Cyber scenarios generally were perceived as worse than traditional ones, although effect sizes were found to be small. These results suggest that the role of medium is secondary to the role of publicity and anonymity when it comes to evaluating bullying severity. Therefore, cyberbullying is not a priori perceived as worse than traditional bullying. Implications of the results for cyberbullying prevention and intervention are discussed.

400 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: An instability of the liquid film against the growth in amplitude of surface waves with a characteristic wavelength is observed, which is believed to be the first observation of spinodal dewetting.
Abstract: We have studied the dewetting of thin liquid metal films (Au, Cu, Ni) on fused silica substrates which occurs after melting with a Q-switched laser pulse. Optical microscopy, scanning electron microscopy, and scanning near field acoustic microscopy reveal two distinctly different modes of the dewetting process: On one hand, we observe heterogeneous nucleation of ``dry'' circular patches, which grow in diameter during the melting period. On the other hand, an instability of the liquid film against the growth in amplitude of surface waves with a characteristic wavelength is observed, which we believe is the first observation of spinodal dewetting. In contrast, the final structure of the ruptured film depends on whether nucleation or spinodal dewetting is dominant.

399 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
TL;DR: Ass associations between belief in conspiracy theories and a range of measures of thinking dispositions and an alternative method of eliciting analytic thinking, which related to cognitive disfluency, was effective at reducing conspiracist ideation in a student sample.

396 citations

Journal ArticleDOI
Richard Karlsson Linnér1, Richard Karlsson Linnér2, Pietro Biroli3, Edward Kong4, S. Fleur W. Meddens2, S. Fleur W. Meddens1, Robbee Wedow, Mark Alan Fontana5, Mark Alan Fontana6, Maël Lebreton7, Stephen P. Tino8, Abdel Abdellaoui1, Anke R. Hammerschlag1, Michel G. Nivard1, Aysu Okbay1, Cornelius A. Rietveld2, Pascal Timshel9, Pascal Timshel10, Maciej Trzaskowski11, Ronald de Vlaming1, Ronald de Vlaming2, Christian L. Zund3, Yanchun Bao12, Laura Buzdugan3, Laura Buzdugan13, Ann H. Caplin, Chia-Yen Chen4, Chia-Yen Chen14, Peter Eibich15, Peter Eibich16, Peter Eibich17, Pierre Fontanillas, Juan R. González18, Peter K. Joshi19, Ville Karhunen20, Aaron Kleinman, Remy Z. Levin21, Christina M. Lill22, Gerardus A. Meddens, Gerard Muntané18, Gerard Muntané23, Sandra Sanchez-Roige21, Frank J. A. van Rooij2, Erdogan Taskesen1, Yang Wu11, Futao Zhang11, Adam Auton, Jason D. Boardman24, David W. Clark19, Andrew Conlin20, Conor C. Dolan1, Urs Fischbacher25, Patrick J. F. Groenen2, Kathleen Mullan Harris26, Gregor Hasler27, Albert Hofman4, Albert Hofman2, Mohammad Arfan Ikram2, Sonia Jain21, Robert Karlsson28, Ronald C. Kessler4, Maarten Kooyman, James MacKillop29, James MacKillop30, Minna Männikkö20, Carlos Morcillo-Suarez18, Matthew B. McQueen24, Klaus M. Schmidt31, Melissa C. Smart12, Matthias Sutter32, Matthias Sutter33, Matthias Sutter16, Roy Thurik2, André G. Uitterlinden2, Jon White34, Harriet de Wit35, Jian Yang11, Lars Bertram22, Lars Bertram36, Dorret I. Boomsma1, Tõnu Esko37, Ernst Fehr3, David A. Hinds, Magnus Johannesson38, Meena Kumari12, David Laibson4, Patrik K. E. Magnusson28, Michelle N. Meyer39, Arcadi Navarro18, Arcadi Navarro40, Abraham A. Palmer21, Tune H. Pers10, Tune H. Pers9, Danielle Posthuma1, Daniel Schunk41, Murray B. Stein21, Rauli Svento20, Henning Tiemeier2, Paul R. H. J. Timmers19, Patrick Turley14, Patrick Turley42, Patrick Turley4, Robert J. Ursano43, Gert G. Wagner17, Gert G. Wagner16, James F. Wilson44, James F. Wilson19, Jacob Gratten11, Jacob Gratten45, James J. Lee46, David Cesarini47, Daniel J. Benjamin48, Daniel J. Benjamin42, Philipp Koellinger1, Philipp Koellinger17, Jonathan P. Beauchamp8 
TL;DR: This paper found evidence of substantial shared genetic influences across risk tolerance and the risky behaviors: 46 of the 99 general risk tolerance loci contain a lead SNP for at least one of their other GWAS, and general risk-tolerance is genetically correlated with a range of risky behaviors.
Abstract: Humans vary substantially in their willingness to take risks. In a combined sample of over 1 million individuals, we conducted genome-wide association studies (GWAS) of general risk tolerance, adventurousness, and risky behaviors in the driving, drinking, smoking, and sexual domains. Across all GWAS, we identified hundreds of associated loci, including 99 loci associated with general risk tolerance. We report evidence of substantial shared genetic influences across risk tolerance and the risky behaviors: 46 of the 99 general risk tolerance loci contain a lead SNP for at least one of our other GWAS, and general risk tolerance is genetically correlated ([Formula: see text] ~ 0.25 to 0.50) with a range of risky behaviors. Bioinformatics analyses imply that genes near SNPs associated with general risk tolerance are highly expressed in brain tissues and point to a role for glutamatergic and GABAergic neurotransmission. We found no evidence of enrichment for genes previously hypothesized to relate to risk tolerance.

395 citations


Authors

Showing all 12272 results

NameH-indexPapersCitations
Robert E. W. Hancock15277588481
Lloyd J. Old152775101377
Andrew White1491494113874
Stefanie Dimmeler14757481658
Rudolf Amann14345985525
Niels Birbaumer14283577853
Thomas P. Russell141101280055
Emmanuelle Perez138155099016
Shlomo Havlin131101383347
Bruno S. Frey11990065368
Roald Hoffmann11687059470
Michael G. Fehlings116118957003
Yves Van de Peer11549461479
Axel Meyer11251151195
Manuela Campanelli11167548563
Network Information
Related Institutions (5)
ETH Zurich
122.4K papers, 5.1M citations

93% related

University of California, Irvine
113.6K papers, 5.5M citations

91% related

Arizona State University
109.6K papers, 4.4M citations

91% related

University of California, Santa Barbara
80.8K papers, 4.6M citations

91% related

Max Planck Society
406.2K papers, 19.5M citations

91% related

Performance
Metrics
No. of papers from the Institution in previous years
YearPapers
202360
2022202
20211,361
20201,299
20191,166
20181,082